Between 14-30% of children and adolescents worldwide have learning difficulties severe enough to require additional support. These difficulties are often associated with cognitive and/or behavioural problems. In some cases, children who are struggling at school receive a formal diagnosis of a specific learning difficulty or disability, such as dyslexia, dyscalculia or developmental language disorder, or of a developmental disorder such as attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), dyspraxia, or autism spectrum disorder.
Scientists have struggled to identify specific areas of the brain that might give rise to these difficulties, with studies implicating myriad brain regions. ADHD, for example, has been linked to the anterior cingulate cortex, caudate nucleus, pallidum, striatum, cerebellum, prefrontal cortex, the premotor cortex and most parts of the parietal lobe.
One potential explanation is that each diagnosis differs so much between one individual and the next, that each involves different combinations of brain regions. However, a more provocative explanation has been proposed by a team of scientists at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge: there are, in fact, no specific brain areas that cause these difficulties.
To test their hypothesis, the researchers used machine learning to map the brain differences across a group of almost 479 children, 337 of whom had been referred with learning-related cognitive problems and 142 from a comparison sample. The algorithm interpreted data taken from a large battery of cognitive, learning, and behavioural measures, as well as from brain scans taken using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). The results are published today in Current Biology.
The researchers found that the brain differences did not map onto any labels the children had been given — in other words, there were no brain regions that predicted having ASD or ADHD. More surprisingly, they found that the different brain regions did not even predict specific cognitive difficulties — there was no specific brain deficit for language problems or memory difficulties.
Instead, the team found that the children’s brains were organised around hubs, like an efficient traffic system or social network. Children who had well-connected brain hubs had either very specific cognitive difficulties, such as poor listening skills, or had no cognitive difficulties at all. By contrast, children with poorly connected hubs — like a train station with few or poor connections — had widespread and severe cognitive problems.
“Scientists have argued for decades that there are specific brain regions that predict having a particular learning disorder or difficulty, but we’ve shown that this isn’t the case,” said Dr Duncan Astle, senior author on the study. “In fact, it’s much more important to consider how these brain areas are connected — specifically, whether they are connected via hubs. The severity of learning difficulties was strongly associated with the connectedness of these hubs, we think because these hubs play a key role in sharing information between brain areas.”
Dr Astle said that one implication of their work is that it suggests that interventions should be less reliant on diagnostic labels.
“Receiving a diagnosis is important for families. It can give professional recognition for a child’s difficulties and open the door to specialist support. But in terms of specific interventions from the child’s teachers, they can be a distraction.
The findings may explain why drugs treatments have not proven effective for developmental disorders. Methylphenidate (Ritalin), for example, which is used to treat ADHD, appears to reduce hyperactivity, but does not remediate cognitive difficulties or improve educational progress. Drugs tend to target specific types of nerve cells, but would have little impact on a ‘hub-based’ organisation that has emerged over many years.
“It’s better to look at their areas of cognitive difficulties and how these can be supported, for example using specific interventions to improve listening skills or language competencies, or at interventions that would be good for the whole class, like how to reduce working memory demands during learning.”
The findings may explain why drugs treatments have not proven effective for developmental disorders. Methylphenidate (Ritalin), for example, which is used to treat ADHD, appears to reduce hyperactivity, but does not remediate cognitive difficulties or improve educational progress. Drugs tend to target specific types of nerve cells, but would have little impact on a ‘hub-based’ organisation that has emerged over many years.
While this is the first time that hubs and their connections have been shown to play a key role in learning difficulties and developmental disorders, their importance in brain disorders is becoming increasingly clear in recent years. Cambridge researchers have previously shown that they also play an important role in mental health disorders that begin to emerge during adolescence, such as schizophrenia.
We quote from the end of the article: “And finally, we only characterize the cognitive and learning impairments of the sample and not their social, environmental, or behavioral profiles. Other highly relevant domains, like home and school life, social processing, and behavior ratings [29], are more challenging to assess reliably and have psychometric properties that are difficult to model. For example, a child’s experiences (environmental interactions, nutrition, education) influence brain development [84] and cognitive test performance [85], but these factors are not measured in the current study.”
Yeah it’s a lot to read… and we skimmed much of it… no question but they CYA’d the science and tried to cover all the variables. A hell of a lot better job than most of the so-called studies of ADHD, ADD, and the like…. In the end, we’d say, “right on” about their conclusions re: special ed – and the diagnoses that propel pupils into questionable learning programs.
Too often the problems that occur with children stem from nutrition, lack of sleep, stress of all sorts, poor parenting, lazy teachers, and the cumulative effects of these and other social actions the child encounters from the get go. And this says nothing about these same effects on Intersex children who are often struggling from additional issues such as gender identity, high empathy, and being bullied.
When we were in elementary school, our refusal to “fight back” because we thought it was stupid, brought our parents to school for poor parenting. Amazing.
What we find disturbing in this is the failure to admit that neuroscientists are still looking at the brain with little or no comprehension of its boundless complexity. As one scientist put, “We are like newcomers to an aquarium drawing life conclusions about fish behavior while watching the in an aquarium.”
LikeLike